The large patches at the dorsal border of medial entorhinal corte

The large patches at the dorsal border of medial entorhinal cortex, however, seem to not have been fully identified NSC 683864 price in previous studies (Witter and Amaral, 2004 and Boccara et al., 2010). Since the medial and dorsal large patches are continuous and cytoarchitectonically similar, we consider them to be one—putatively parasubicular—structure and refer to them as large patches. Often but not

always the large patches could be divided in two vertically split subpatches (Figures 2A and 2B). Quantification of cytochrome oxidase activity levels revealed a clear periodicity of patches (Figures S2A and S2B), which were visible along the entire mediolateral extent of medial entorhinal cortex (Figures S2C–S2E). To further characterize the organization of medial entorhinal cortex, we stained alternating parasagittal, horizontal, or tangential sections for cytochrome oxidase activity, Nissl, and myelin. Differences in cell size, density, soma morphology, and cytochrome oxidase activity confirmed the existence of the two types of patches (Figures 2A–2C). Areas of higher cell density in layer 2, as visualized by Nissl staining, coincided with the patches identified by cytochrome oxidase activity staining (Figure 2C), and the patchy organization was typically

more obvious in cytochrome oxidase than in Nissl stains. Large patches showed strong cytochrome oxidase reactivity, probably reflecting

a constitutively high metabolic activity. They differed strikingly from the surrounding cortical sheet and distorted the cortical lamination (Figure 2D). Fasudil cell line Their broad PTPRJ dorsal part extended into layer 1, and their ventral part tapered out toward layer 4. Many myelinated axons originated from these patches, but myelination did not extend into their broad dorsal part (Figure 2E). The small layer 2 patches were also often surrounded by myelinated fibers (data not shown). The architecture of small patches changed along the dorsoventral axis: cell size and myelination decreased (data not shown), while patch size increased (Figure 2F; Figure S3). Cells in large patches appeared to be smaller than adjacent neurons in small layer 2 patches (Figure 2D) and had a unique dendritic morphology, strongly polarized away from the patch border (Figure 2G). Within layer 2 the dendrites of layer 2 stellate cells were also largely but not exclusively restricted to their home patch, while they extended more broadly in layer 1 (Figure 2G; Figure S4). Interestingly, patch diameters seemed to be within the range of the deep-to-superficial “input clusters” widths reported by Beed et al. (2010) for stellate cells (∼200 μm at midlevel of medial entorhinal cortex), suggesting a possible correlation between patches and interlaminar inputs in medial entorhinal cortex.

Since GlialCAM has been described to target MLC1 to cell-cell

Since GlialCAM has been described to target MLC1 to cell-cell

junctions (López-Hernández et al., 2011b), we assayed if GlialCAM could also modify ClC-2 localization in the same manner. In HeLa cells, ClC-2 transfected alone was detected at the plasma membrane and intracellularly (Figure 3A). Coexpression with GlialCAM directed the ClC-2 channel to cell-cell contacts (Figures 3B–3D), where both proteins colocalized (data not shown). Localization of ClC-2 together with GlialCAM was observed in long (Figure 3B) or short (Figure 3C) cell-cell contact processes and in extensive contact areas between opposite cells (Figure 3D). Such a http://www.selleckchem.com/products/Trichostatin-A.html clustering was never observed in contacting cells expressing only ClC-2 (Figure 3A). Similar results were observed in HEK293 cells (data not shown). We performed analogous experiments in primary cultures of astrocytes, where both proteins are endogenously expressed. In these cultures, adenoviral-mediated expression of ClC-2 with or without GlialCAM showed that the latter protein was necessary to target ClC-2

to astrocyte-astrocyte processes (compare Figures 3E and 3F). In these junctions, ClC-2 and GlialCAM displayed colocalization (Figures 3F–3H). We next asked whether GlialCAM could modify ClC-2 function. Coexpression of GlialCAM Nivolumab price and ClC-2 in Xenopus oocytes dramatically increased ClC-2-mediated currents ifoxetine and changed their characteristics ( Figure 4A). Initial currents measured at +60 mV were more than 15-fold larger in cells coexpressing ClC-2 and GlialCAM compared to ClC-2 alone. Whereas ClC-2 currents are strongly inwardly rectifying and activate slowly upon hyperpolarization, ClC-2/GlialCAM currents were almost ohmic and displayed time-independent, instantaneously active currents ( Figure 4B). Of note, the apparent inactivation observed sometimes at very negative voltages

is an artifact caused by chloride depletion inside the oocytes. Similar effects of GlialCAM on ClC-2 currents were seen in transfected HEK293 cells, although a residual time-dependent component was present (Figure 4C). Importantly, GlialCAM alone does not induce any significant current in HEK cells or Xenopus oocytes ( Figure S4). Similarly, in transfected cells, ClC-2 steady state currents at +60 mV were dramatically increased by GlialCAM ( Figure 4D). Specificity of the currents was demonstrated by the characteristic block by extracellular iodide ( Gründer et al., 1992 and Thiemann et al., 1992; Figure 4B) and cadmium ( Clark et al., 1998) (data not shown). To test if GlialCAM may alter native ClC-2 currents we performed whole-cell patch-clamp experiments in differentiated rat astrocytes. These cells exhibit typical hyperpolarization-activated ClC-2-like currents that were blocked by iodide (Ferroni et al., 1997 and Makara et al., 2003; Figure 4E).

Our correlation-based intrinsic functional connectivity approache

Our correlation-based intrinsic functional connectivity approaches Saracatinib cost only measure symmetric (undirected) connections between regions with temporally synchronous BOLD fluctuations. These methods cannot

differentiate direct from indirect links or infer causality (direction of information flow). These limitations apply to all current intrinsic functional network analyses in humans because the true graph (determined at the microscopic level by the presence of axonal connections between regions) cannot be determined with existing methods. We attempted to mitigate these concerns by thresholding the graphs at a stringent statistical threshold, leaving only strong edges for calculation of graph metrics, but this approach does not preclude our edges from representing indirect connections within or outside the network. Despite these limitations, the functional network graphs derived here provide relevant data about network organization. Understanding the cellular and molecular basis for network-based disease spread represents an important priority for neurodegenerative disease research. Human intrinsic connectivity data cannot directly inform cellular pathogenesis models, just as simple laboratory models include assumptions regarding

their relevance to human disease. This study sought to bridge these research streams by translating mechanistic network-based neurodegeneration models into simple but rational predictions PLX3397 in vivo regarding the relationships secondly between network connectivity and vulnerability. Complementary studies using structural connectivity data could further explore connectivity-vulnerability interactions. The present findings suggest that, overall, a transneuronal spread model best accounts for the

network-based vulnerability observed in previous human neuropathological and imaging studies. Several mechanisms of transneuronal spread have been proposed, including axonal transport of undetected viruses or toxins (Hawkes et al., 2007 and Saper et al., 1987). Providing a more parsimonious account, growing evidence suggests that prion-like mechanisms may promote the spread of toxic, misfolded, nonprion protein species between interconnected neurons (Baker et al., 1993, Baker et al., 1994, Brundin et al., 2010, Clavaguera et al., 2009, Frost and Diamond, 2010, Frost et al., 2009, Hansen et al., 2011, Jucker and Walker, 2011, Lee et al., 2010, Li et al., 2008, Ridley et al., 2006 and Walker et al., 2006). This notion, that many or all noninfectious neurodegenerative diseases may propagate along networked axons via templated conformational change, has been put forth since the introduction of the prion concept (Prusiner, 1984 and a).

We scanned alert monkeys while they passively viewed 20 s blocks<

We scanned alert monkeys while they passively viewed 20 s blocks

of Learned symbols, Untrained shapes (other human symbols differing in shape from the Learned symbols), and Faces, alternating with 20 s blocks of a small fixation spot (Figure 3). We first calculated maximum likelihood HSP assay maps of responsiveness to each stimulus category (Learned symbols, Untrained shapes, Faces) using general linear model methods (Boynton et al., 1996), wherein a hemodynamic impulse response function was convolved with the stimulus paradigm. We defined three category contrasts by performing t tests between responses to different pairs of stimulus categories: Learned symbols versus Faces (LvsF), Learned symbols versus Untrained shapes (LvsU), and Faces versus Untrained shapes (FvsU). Then we defined three category selectivity maps using a conjunction analysis ( Bell et al., 2009 and Price et al., 1997) on the three contrast conditions, using odd-numbered scans:

Face-selective voxels were defined as being more responsive to both F > U AND F > L, both contrasts p < 0.001 (corrected for multiple comparisons, see methods), Shape-selective regions satisfied both L > F AND U > F at p < 0.001, and Learned symbol-selective regions satisfied RO4929097 datasheet both L > U AND L > F at p < 0.001. The maps in Figure 4 and Figure 5 show these category-selective regions, projected onto semi-inflated anatomical maps for each monkey. In all six monkeys, several bilateral regions of the inferior temporal lobe were Carnitine palmitoyltransferase II more active to Faces than to either shape category (F > U AND F > L), consistent with previous reports of face selective regions in the temporal lobe (Tsao et al., 2003). These Face-selective regions showed >90% overlap between the left and right hemispheres for all six monkeys (see Table S1 available online); therefore, we averaged together the left and right Face-selective activations. We identified the three largest Face patches in each monkey as f1, f2, and f3 (posterior to anterior).

We projected the Face-selective patches from each individual monkey onto a common semi-inflated left hemisphere (Figure 6A, red patches); the patches were roughly overlapping in this projection, indicating some consistency in location from monkey to monkey, except for the most anterior Face region, which could comprise two patches or may simply be less reproducible in location from monkey to monkey. The location of the maximally selective voxels in each of the Face-selective patches in each monkey are given in Table S1. The most posterior Face patch (f1) was located in posterior area TEO, sometimes extending into anterior V4, on the ventral bank of the STS near the anterior tip of IOS, with the region of maximum overlap between monkeys at A1. The middle Face patch (f2) was mostly in area TEa with the region of maximum overlap at A8.

To break the CS-US contingency, Pavlov developed an experimental

To break the CS-US contingency, Pavlov developed an experimental procedure in which the CS was presented alone (without the US) for several trials after the completion of conditioning (Pavlov, 1927). Not surprisingly, the earliest CS-alone trials produced a robust CR, but the CR gradually faded with subsequent CS presentations. Pavlov termed this phenomenon “extinction,” and it is now apparent that this form of learning is an important component of behavioral interventions for patients with pathological fear memories. For example, exposure therapy involves

the use of mental imagery and exposure to trauma-relevant cues in a safe environment to suppress the fear associated with the memory of the traumatic event (Craske et al., 2008, Powers et al., 2010 and Rothbaum OSI-744 and Davis, 2003). Given the importance of extinction learning as a mechanism for suppressing fear memory, there has been an explosion of work into the neural mechanisms of extinction (Bouton et al., 2006a, Herry et al., 2010, Myers and Davis, 2002, Pape and Pare, 2010 and Quirk and Mueller, 2008). Not surprisingly, much of this work has focused on the contribution of the amygdala to fear Selleckchem Autophagy inhibitor extinction and several reports indicate that the BLA is critical for the acquisition of

extinction memories. For example, infusing NMDA receptor antagonists into the BLA disrupts the acquisition of extinction (Falls et al., 1992,

Laurent et al., 2008 and Zimmerman and Maren, 2010), whereas blockade of NMDA receptors in the CEA does not affect extinction learning (Zimmerman and Maren, 2010). Intracellular signaling pathways downstream of BLA NMDA receptors are also critical for extinction learning (Herry et al., 2006, Lin et al., 2003a, Lin et al., 2003b, Lu et al., 2001 and Yang and Lu, 2005). In addition to the glutamatergic system, recent work indicates that other neurotransmitter systems contribute to extinction learning. For example, mice lacking endocannabinoid receptors (CB1 receptors, specifically) exhibit impairments in extinction learning and systemic administration of a CB1 antagonist (SR141716, rimonabant) Sitaxentan inhibits extinction learning (Chhatwal et al., 2009 and Marsicano et al., 2002). Endocannabinoids modulate inhibitory GABAergic synaptic transmission in the amygdala, which is also essential for extinction learning (Chhatwal et al., 2005b, Harris and Westbrook, 1998, Laurent et al., 2008, Laurent and Westbrook, 2008 and Makkar et al., 2010). Collectively, these data suggest that changes in synaptic transmission within the BLA contribute to the suppression of conditional fear after extinction training. Indeed, depotentiation of amygdaloid synaptic transmission has been reported to occur after extinction training (Kim et al., 2007).

There are also differences between SVZ and SGZ neurogenesis in sp

There are also differences between SVZ and SGZ neurogenesis in specific aspects, mainly in the niche organization, neuronal subtype differentiation, and migration Selleck GSK1120212 of newborn neurons. Adult neurogenesis recapitulates many features of embryonic neurogenesis. Indeed, the adult neurogenesis field has benefit tremendously from our knowledge of embryonic neurogenesis, such as the role of classic morphogens and transcription factors. Genetic analysis of adult neurogenesis is generally challenging and requires inducible and conditional approaches to ensure normal embryonic and early postnatal development. On the

other hand, because of its relative simplicity, adult neurogenesis may

provide an optimal system to investigate underlying molecular mechanisms and explore functions of susceptibility genes for mental disorders in neuronal development (reviewed by Christian et al., 2010). Indeed, some novel pathways were first identified in adult neurogenesis and later shown to be conserved in embryonic development (Cancedda et al., 2007 and Ge et al., 2006). Future comparative studies of embryonic and adult neurogenesis will remain to be fruitful. Significant questions still remain to be addressed regarding clonal properties of adult neural precursor subtypes, organization of the niche, cellular and molecular mechanisms regulating different aspects of neurogenesis see more under basal and stimulated conditions, contributions of new neurons to normal and aberrant brain functions, and properties

and functions of human adult neurogenesis. We also need to have a better understanding whether there are causal relationships between adult neurogenesis and animal behavior and between defects in adult neurogenesis and symptoms of degenerative neurological disorders. The Levetiracetam presence of functional adult neurogenesis throughout life demonstrates the strikingly plastic nature of the adult mammalian brain. While we focused our discussion on newborn neurons, it is important to appreciate that the adult CNS environment is also permissive for continuous structural rearrangement and development of adult-born neurons and that mature neurons can be extremely plastic as they constantly form new functional synaptic connections with adult-born neurons. Given the lack of effective regeneration after injury for neurons in the adult mammalian CNS (reviewed by Kim et al., 2006), more effort needs to be devoted to investigate the plastic nature of the adult CNS in general. Building upon the exciting recent progress and development of new tools, the adult neurogenesis field is poised to make another giant leap forward.

Drifting gratings with six orientations (12 directions) were pres

Drifting gratings with six orientations (12 directions) were presented to examine the orientation selectivity of F+ and F− cells. Response magnitude (ΔF/F) in response to the drifting gratings, orientation selectivity index (OSI; see Experimental Procedures), and tuning width (see Experimental Procedures) was not significantly different between F+ and F− cells (p > 0.1; Kolmogorov-Smirnov test; Figures S2A–S2C). We found that sister cells tended to be tuned to similar orientations. In seven of eight clones that we examined, more than 50% of sister

cells had preferred orientations within 40° of each other. Figure 2 shows a representative experiment. Time courses of calcium indicator during visual stimulation were recorded from OGB-1-loaded cells with two-photon selleck compound microscopy (Figure 2B). Of 142 F+ cells recorded from layers 2–4 (Figure 2A), 111 cells showed a significant response to the ON-01910 in vivo drifting gratings (p < 0.01, ANOVA across 12 directions and a baseline; ΔF/F > 2%; see Experimental Procedures) and 68 cells showed

orientation selectivity (p < 0.01, ANOVA across six orientations). Of these, 28 cells were sharply selective for orientation (tuning width, half width at half maximum < 45°), and we used only these cells for further analyses. More than half (18/28) of these F+ cells preferred gratings with vertical orientation (−5° to +30°; Figure 2B, orange; Figure 3A, top), although ten other F+ cells preferred other orientations (Figure 2B, green), so that more than half

of sister cells were tuned to similar orientations within 35° of each other. However, we found that even the nearby nonclonally related F− cells with sharp orientation selectivity showed PRKACG some bias for preferred orientation (Figure 3A, bottom), as has been reported previously in mouse visual cortex (Ohki et al., 2005 and Kreile et al., 2011). A bias of similar magnitude was also observed in C57BL/6 wild-type mice (Figures S3A and S3B). To precisely quantify this bias in wild-type animals, we repeated these measurements in C57BL/6 wild-type mice (n = 7) under very similar experimental conditions and confirmed that the magnitude of the bias in our transgenic mice (n = 8) is similar to that in C57BL/6 wild-type mice (n = 7) by quantifying the magnitude of the bias with Fourier analysis (p > 0.5; Kolmogorov-Smirnov test; see legend of Figure S3). After pooling histograms from all the examples from transgenic (n = 8) and wild-type (n = 7) mice, the histograms (Figures S3C and S3D) were similar to those previously reported (Kreile et al., 2011). Because local populations in visual cortex can have overall biases in their preferred orientations, a small number of randomly chosen cells can have similar orientation tuning just by chance.

In the AVM cell, AHR-1 elevates MEC-3 expression as well

In the AVM cell, AHR-1 elevates MEC-3 expression as well

as blocks downstream Paclitaxel in vivo MEC-3 targets that result in traits normally reserved for PVD (e.g., lateral branching, sensitivity to low temperatures). Thus, AHR-1 is required for the twinned tasks of inducing the light touch fate while simultaneously preventing expression of nociceptor genes. We show that one of these targets, the claudin-like membrane protein HPO-30, acts in PVD to stabilize lateral dendrites. We hypothesize that HPO-30/claudin maintains PVD dendritic branches by mediating adhesive interactions with the adjacent epidermis. HPO-30 is ectopically expressed in the ahr-1 mutant AVM cell and is required for its PVD-like morphology. We note that this effect is remarkably similar to that of the mutant phenotype for the Drosophila AHR-1 homolog, Spineless,

in which simple sensory neurons adopt more complex arbors, although the Spineless targets that effect this outcome are not known ( Kim et al., 2006). The strong conservation of this role in dendritic branching suggests that the vertebrate Spineless homolog is likely to exercise a similar function, and thus that the downstream effector molecules that we have identified in C. elegans may also pattern the architecture of mammalian sensory neurons. C. elegans responds to physical stimuli through a diverse array of mechanosensory neurons ( Chatzigeorgiou et al., 2010b, Geffeney SB431542 et al., 2011, Chalfie and Sulston, 1981 and Hall and Treinin, 2011). Light touch

to the body (posterior to pharynx) is mediated by six TRNs (AVM, PVM, PLML, PLMR, ALMR, and ALML), whereas a harsh mechanical stimulus to this region is detected by PVDL and PVDR ( Figure 1) ( Way and Chalfie, 1989). These neurons occupy unique locations and adopt distinct branching patterns. The touch receptor neurons display a simple morphology with unbranched longitudinal processes emanating from the cell soma. In contrast, the “harsh-touch” PVD MRIP neurons are highly branched with elaborate dendritic arbors that envelop the animal in a net-like array ( Figure 1) ( Halevi et al., 2002, Oren-Suissa et al., 2010, Smith et al., 2010 and Tsalik et al., 2003). FLP neurons in the head, which also respond to harsh mechanical force ( Chatzigeorgiou and Schafer, 2011), show a similar PVD-like pattern of orthogonal dendritic branches ( Albeg et al., 2011 and Smith et al., 2010). PVD displays additional sensory responses to temperature and hyperosmolarity ( Chatzigeorgiou et al., 2010b) (shown later in Figure 4). The members of these subgroups of mechanosensory neurons are also distinguished by their developmental origins. The touch neurons ALMR, ALML, PLMR, and PLML are generated in the embryo ( Sulston et al., 1983). AVM and PVM are each produced during the first larval (L1) stage by unique patterns of cell migration and division of Q-cell progenitors on the left (PVM) and right (AVM) sides of the body ( Sulston and Horvitz, 1977).

09) Consistent with these observations, we also observed that ex

09). Consistent with these observations, we also observed that experience led to decreases in the proportion of stimuli eliciting a significant elevation in firing rate and to increases in the proportion of stimuli eliciting a significant reduction in firing rate (Figure S4). Furthermore, although both cell classes showed reduced average responses to familiar stimuli, this R428 ic50 decrease was much larger in putative inhibitory than excitatory cells (early epoch, p = 0.001; late epoch, p < 0.001; two-sample t tests; early epoch effect not significant in the same monkey whose

effects tended to arise later), which can be seen by comparing the red and blue arrows in the histograms of Figures 4C and 4D. To convey information, neurons modulate their firing rates. The greater and/or more reliable this modulation, the more informative the neuron’s firing rate becomes about the presence (or absence) of some stimulus. Because we have shown that visual experience not only led to an increase in maximum response (in putative excitatory cells) but also to a decrease in average response, we have already implicated visual experience in sharper stimulus selectivity. Here, we make this idea explicit. To capture increases in selectivity with a single metric, we computed the value of (lifetime) sparseness (Olshausen and Field, 2004, Rolls and Tovee, 1995,

Vinje DNA Damage inhibitor and Gallant, 2000 and Zoccolan et al., 2007) (see Experimental Procedures). Sparseness quantifies how much of a single neuron’s total firing rate, across a stimulus set, is concentrated within a few stimuli. A neuron with high sparseness will be quiet

most of the time, but there will be a few stimuli that elicit robust firing rates. By definition, this is a selective neuron. An unselective neuron, one with low sparseness, will respond with an elevated firing rate to many stimuli. We calculated the sparseness of cells’ responses across the familiar and novel stimulus sets, first with a sliding window (Figures 5A and 5B) and then in the previously defined early and late epochs (Figures 5C and 5D). As with the average response analyses, one of the more conspicuous features of the data was that putative inhibitory units had much lower sparseness than putative excitatory Cell Penetrating Peptide units for every combination of stimulus set and epoch (mean ± SEM putative excitatory versus putative inhibitory; familiar early, 0.53 ± 0.03 versus 0.16 ± 0.02; familiar late, 0.65 ± 0.03 versus 0.32 ± 0.04; novel early, 0.42 ± 0.02 versus 0.17 ± 0.02; novel late, 0.57 ± 0.02 versus 0.24 ± 0.02; p < 0.001 for every comparison, uncorrected, two-sample t tests). The broad tuning of putative inhibitory units is consistent with recent functional data (Kerlin et al., 2010, Liu et al., 2009 and Sohya et al., 2007) as well as neuroanatomical data showing that these units can receive highly convergent and heterogeneous input from the surrounding excitatory population (Bock et al., 2011).

For example,

For example, Nutlin-3a cost in the olfactory domain, a hunting dog may require multiple sniffs to decide whether a fast-moving rabbit has darted left or right under a hedgerow; a human may take several sniffs to decide whether a carton of milk on the verge of spoiling is a wise

breakfast option. The implication is that the nervous system accumulates sensory information over time for efficient perceptual decision-making. Neuroscientific support for the integration of noisy perceptual evidence is principally based on single-unit studies in nonhuman animals (Gold and Shadlen, 2007; Newsome et al., 1989; Platt, 2002; Romo and Salinas, 2001; Schall and Thompson, 1999). In a widely studied visual perceptual paradigm (Cook and Maunsell, 2002; Hanes and Schall, 1996;

Newsome et al., 1989; Platt and Glimcher, 1999), responses in the lateral intraparietal area (LIP) show a ramp-like increase during a dot-motion discrimination task, such that animals make a decision when neuronal activity surpasses a bound (Roitman and Shadlen, 2002; Shadlen and Newsome, 2001). Such findings have helped inform and constrain models of perceptual decision-making. Human imaging studies have begun using simple two-choice tasks to explore the neural substrates of visual perceptual decision-making (Heekeren et al., 2004; Huettel et al., 2005; Ivanoff et al., 2008; Noppeney et al., 2010; Ploran et al., 2007; Tosoni et al., 2008). However, the direct integration of perceptual evidence over time and Obeticholic Acid clinical trial its modulation by the degree Histone demethylase of sensory noise are poorly understood. Resolving temporal integration using functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) is difficult because

humans tend to solve perceptual tasks much faster than the minimum data-acquisition rate of functional MRI scanners—too few data points are obtained per trial to allow the characterization of signal integration during the decision process. Traditional wisdom thus holds that fMRI is too slow to capture sensory integration (Noppeney et al., 2010; Philiastides and Sajda, 2007). Here we took advantage of the fact that human olfactory perception evolves at a slow timescale, particularly for mixtures of odorants (Laing and Francis, 1989). This natural prolongation of response times implies that the olfactory system is ideally suited to characterize perceptual evidence integration with imaging techniques. In this study, we used fMRI to measure brain activity while subjects participated in a two-choice olfactory categorization task. Varying the relative proportion of components in a two-odorant mixture (Abraham et al., 2004; Boyle et al., 2009; Kepecs et al., 2008; Khan et al., 2008; Rinberg et al., 2006; Uchida and Mainen, 2003; Wesson et al., 2008) allowed us to manipulate odor mixture difficulty and to titrate the speed and accuracy of decision-making. With a combination of model-based fMRI approaches (O’Doherty et al., 2007), olfactory psychophysics, and deconvolution techniques (Glover, 1999; Zelano et al.